adplus-dvertising


jocn a 00459 PDF


À propos / Télécharger Lire le document
Nom original: jocn_a_00459.pdf
Titre: jcn00459 96..106

Ce fichier au format PDF 1.2 a été généré par Arbortext Advanced Print Publisher 9.0.114/W Unicode / Acrobat Distiller 4.05 for Windows, et a été envoyé sur Fichier-PDF le 29/11/2013 à 17:45, depuis l'adresse IP 89.224.x.x. La présente page de téléchargement du fichier a été vue 821 fois.
Taille du document: 483 Ko (11 pages).
Confidentialité: fichier public





Aperçu du document


The Shape of the ACC Contributes to Cognitive
Control Efficiency in Preschoolers
Arnaud Cachia1,2*, Grégoire Borst1,2*, Julie Vidal1,2, Clara Fischer3,
Arlette Pineau4, Jean-François Mangin3, and Olivier Houdé1,2,5

Abstract
■ Cognitive success at school and later in life is supported by

executive functions including cognitive control (CC). The pFC
plays a major role in CC, particularly the dorsal part of ACC or
midcingulate cortex. Genes, environment (including school curricula), and neuroplasticity affect CC. However, no study to date
has investigated whether ACC sulcal pattern, a stable brain feature
primarily determined in utero, influences CC efficiency in the
early stages of cognitive and neural development. Using anatomical MRI and three-dimensional reconstruction of cortical folds,
we investigated the effect that ACC sulcal pattern may have on
the Stroop score, a classical behavioral index of CC efficiency, in
5-year-old preschoolers. We found higher CC efficiency, that is,

INTRODUCTION
Cognitive control (CC) including inhibitory control—that
is, the ability to overcome conflicts and inhibit a dominant
response—is one of the core executive functions that
enable us to resist habits or automatisms, temptations,
distractions, or interference and allow us to adapt to
complex situations by means of mental flexibility, namely,
dynamic inhibition/activation of competing cognitive
strategies (Diamond, 2013). The Stroop Color–Word task
(Stroop, 1935) is a seminal task designed to assess the
ability to process conflicting information, drawing, in part,
on CC. In the classical Stroop Color–Word task, participants are instructed to name the color of the ink of
printed words that denote colors. In the no-conflict condition, the ink colors match the colors denoted by the
words (e.g., “RED” appears in red ink), whereas in the
conflict condition, the colors denoted by the words interfere with the ink colors to be named (e.g., “RED” appears
in blue ink). The conflict condition, in contrast to the no-

1

CNRS U3521, Laboratory for the Psychology of Child Development and Education, Sorbonne, Paris, France, 2Université Paris
Descartes, Paris, France, 3Computer-Assisted Neuroimaging
Laboratory, Neurospin, I2BM, CEA, Gif/ Yvette, France, 4Université
Caen Basse Normandie, Caen, France, 5Institut Universitaire de
France, Paris, France
*These authors contributed equally to this work.
© 2013 Massachusetts Institute of Technology

lower Stroop interference scores for both RTs and error rates,
in children with asymmetrical ACC sulcal pattern (i.e., different
pattern in each hemisphere) compared with children with symmetrical pattern (i.e., same pattern in both hemispheres). Critically, ACC sulcal pattern had no effect on performance in the
forward and backward digit span tasks suggesting that ACC sulcal
pattern contributes to the executive ability to resolve conflicts
but not to the ability to maintain and manipulate information in
working memory. This finding provides the first evidence that
preschoolersʼ CC efficiency is likely associated with ACC sulcal
pattern, thereby suggesting that the brain shape could result in
early constraints on human executive ability. ■

conflict condition, typically results in increased RTs and
error rates (ERs) because of the need to inhibit irrelevant
stimulus feature (i.e., the color denoted by the word) to
focus on an alternative feature of the stimulus (i.e., the
ink color). The Stroop interference score reflect the ability
of CC to overcome perceptual and cognitive conflicts
through the inhibition of a dominant response, namely
reading when verbal material is presented (MacLeod,
1991). However, the Stroop Color–Word task involves
other cognitive processes, such as selective attention, conflict monitoring, perceptual, semantic interference, response interference, and working memory (MacLeod,
Dodd, Sheard, Wilson, & Bibi, 2003). Functional brainimaging studies (Roberts & Hall, 2008; Matthews, Paulus,
Simmons, Nelesen, & Dimsdale, 2004; Bush, Luu, &
Posner, 2000; Casey et al., 2000; Pardo, Pardo, Janer, &
Raichle, 1990) have demonstrated that the medial pFC
and more precisely the dorsal ACC, also referred to as the
midcingulate cortex (Vogt, 2009), is consistently activated
in Stroop tasks (Petersen & Posner, 2012) and other tasks
that involve overriding prepotent responses, selecting responses in underdetermined contexts, or errors (Petersen
& Posner, 2012). According to the conflict-monitoring
hypothesis (Botvinick, 2007; Botvinick, Braver, Barch,
Carter, & Cohen, 2001), one of the core functions of the
dorsal ACC is to signal conflict in information processing
to the CC system supported through dorsolateral prefrontal
cortices. To resolve this conflict, the CC system increases
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 26:1, pp. 96–106
doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00459

the activation of task-relevant information and inhibits
task-irrelevant information (see Egner & Hirsch, 2005).
From a developmental psychology perspective, executive functions including CC are known to support
qualities such as self-control, creativity, and reasoning
that children require to be successful in school and later
in life (Diamond, 2013). Executive function efficiency is a
better predictor of school readiness and future academic
success than intelligence quotient (Blair & Razza, 2007;
Duckworth & Seligman, 2005). Given the critical role
that executive functions play in academic achievement,
numerous studies have focused on how to improve
executive function efficiency. Diverse activities seem
to increase executive function efficiency (Diamond &
Lee, 2011), including school curricula (Diamond, Barnett,
Thomas, & Munro, 2007), attention training (Rueda,
Rothbart, McCandliss, Saccomanno, & Posner, 2005),
computerized training (Holmes, Gathercole, & Dunning,
2009), noncomputerized games (Mackey, Hill, Stone, &
Bunge, 2011), aerobics (Hillman, Erickson, & Kramer,
2008), martial arts (Lakes & Hoyt, 2004), yoga, and mindfulness (Flook et al., 2010). For instance, Tools of the Mind—
a school curriculum for preschool kindergarten that emphasizes imaginary play—improves executive functions to a
larger extent than high-quality school curricula based on
literacy and thematic units (Diamond et al., 2007).
From a neuroscience perspective, studies have demonstrated that prolonged learning and specific trainings—
leading to the improvement of cognitive efficiency—can
modify the structure (e.g., gray matter volume, cortical
thickness) of brain areas functionally related to the processes trained (Hyde et al., 2009; Draganski et al., 2004,
2006). For example, adults who learned to juggle over
a 3-month period present an increase of the gray matter
volume in the visual motion area. The increase of the gray
matter volume reveals one of the neuroplasticity mechanisms mediating the improvement of cognitive ability
following intense training (Draganski et al., 2004). This is
not limited to the motor domain; studies have demonstrated variation of the structure of the brain in response
to intense learning for medical examinations (Draganski
et al., 2006) and in response to 15 months of musical training in early childhood—with a correlation between the
structural brain changes induced by training and behavioral improvements (Hyde et al., 2009). Although there
is no study to date that has investigated the structural
change in ACC in response to CC training, previous studies
have provided evidence that interindividual differences
in adultsʼ CC efficiency are associated with differences
in the structure of ACC, that is, the local cortical thickness
(Westlye, Grydeland, Walhovd, & Fjell, 2011) or the local
gray matter volume (Takeuchi et al., 2012) of ACC.
Early determined anatomical features of ACC also contribute to interindividual differences in adultsʼ CC efficiency.
The sulcal pattern constitutes one of these early anatomical factors ( Welker, 1988). This pattern is determined
in utero by genetic and environmental factors (Barkovich,

Guerrini, Kuzniecky, Jackson, & Dobyns, 2012; Rakic,
2004; Molko et al., 2003; Dehay, Giroud, Berland, Killackey,
& Kennedy, 1996). As opposed to quantitative measures
of the cortex morphology, such as the Gyrification Index
(Zilles, Palomero-Gallagher, & Amunts, 2013; White, Su,
Schmidt, Kao, & Sapiro, 2010; Armstrong, Schleicher,
Omran, Curtis, & Zilles, 1995) or cortex structure, such
as the thickness, surface, and volume (Giedd et al., 2009;
Gogtay et al., 2004), that vary from childhood through
early adulthood, the sulcal pattern, a qualitative measure
of the cortex morphology, is a stable feature of the brain
anatomy apparently less affected by brain maturation,
training, and learning occurring after birth (Sun et al.,
2012). Two types of ACC sulcal pattern (Ono, Kubik, &
Abarnathey, 1990) are defined between 10 and 15 weeks
of fetal life (White et al., 2010): the “single” type, with
only the cingulate sulcus, and the “double parallel” type,
with an additional paracingulate sulcus (PCS; Paus et al.,
1996). Recent functional data indicate that participants
without a PCS do not lack a particular cortical area, that
is, “single” and `“double parallel” ACC types are homologous cortical regions (Amiez et al., 2013). The “double
parallel” is observed in 30–60% of individuals, and this
ACC type is more frequent in the left hemisphere (Yucel
et al., 2001). In adults, asymmetry in the sulcal pattern of
ACC (i.e., the “single” type in the left hemisphere and
the “double parallel” type in the right hemisphere or vice
versa) is associated with increased CC efficiency (Huster,
Westerhausen, & Herrmann, 2011; Fornito et al., 2004)
and the increased efficiency to manage cognitive conflicts
and inhibit dominant responses as measured by the performance in a Stroop Color–Word task at the behavioral
and electrophysiological level (Huster, Enriquez-Geppert,
Pantev, & Bruchmann, 2012; Huster et al., 2009). However,
no study to date has investigated whether the sulcal
pattern of ACC already affects CC efficiency in the early
stages of cognitive and neural development.
In our study, using anatomical MRI, we investigated
whether an early neurodevelopmental constraint—that
is, the sulcal pattern of ACC—contributes to preschoolersʼ
CC efficiency as measured by their performance on
the “Animal Stroop task” ( Wright, Waterman, Prescott,
& Murdoch-Eaton, 2003)—an adaptation of the Stroop
Color–Word task for young nonreading children. In the
“Animal Stroop task” (Figure 1), children are required to
name an animalʼs body in a no-conflict condition—that
is, the head and the animalʼs body are matched (e.g.,
a duckʼs head on a duckʼs body)—and in a conflict condition—that is, the head of the animal is replaced by the
head of a different animal (e.g., a pigʼs head on a duckʼs
body). As in the classical Stroop Color–Word task, CC
efficiency is reflected by the difference in RTs (or ERs)
between the conflict and the no-conflict conditions.
If the sulcal pattern of ACC contributes to preschoolersʼ
CC efficiency, we expect lower Stroop interference
scores (i.e., better CC efficiency) for children with asymmetric (i.e., the “single” type in the left hemisphere and
Cachia et al.

97

Figure 1. Assessment of CC
efficiency using the “Animal
Stroop task.” (Left) A child
performing the “Animal Stroop
task” in the classroom. (Right)
Example of “conflict” and
“no-conflict” stimuli used in
the “Animal Stroop task” to
assess the CC efficiency of
preschoolers.

the “double parallel” type in the right hemisphere or
vice versa) rather than symmetric (i.e., the “single” type
or “double parallel” type in both hemispheres) sulcal
pattern of ACC (Figure 2).
In addition, to assess the specificity of the effect of
ACC sulcal pattern on the CC efficiency of preschoolers,
the same group of children performed both forward and
backward digit span tasks from the Wechsler Intelligence
Scale for Children ( WISC-IV; see Wechsler, 2003). We
reasoned that if ACC sulcal pattern contributes specifically to the CC efficiency and not to the efficiency of
other executive functions, such as verbal working memory, then ACC sulcal pattern should have no effect on the
performance in the two-digit span tasks, even on the
backward digit span task, which requires more executive

load (Gathercole, Pickering, Ambridge, & Wearing, 2004;
Baddeley, 2003).

METHODS
Participants
Nineteen 5-year-old right-handed preschoolers (mean ±
standard deviation age: 5.47 ± 0.18 years; 11 girls) were
recruited from a public preschool in Caen (France).
They had no history of neurological disease and no
cerebral abnormalities. The children were tested in accordance with national and international norms that
govern the use of human research participants. We obtained written informed consent from the childrenʼs
parents that allowed us to enroll their children in the
study. The ethics committee approved our study (CPP
Nord-Ouest III, France).
Behavioral Assessment

Figure 2. Morphological patterns of ACC. The two ACC sulcal patterns:
“single” type, with only the cingulate sulcus, and “double parallel”
type, with an additional PCS. ACC sulci (blue) are represented on
the cortical surface (gray/white interface).

98

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

The preschoolersʼ CC efficiency was assessed using the
“Animal Stroop task,” an adaptation of the Stroop Color–
Word task for preschoolers ( Wright et al., 2003). Each
preschooler performed two experimental conditions—
each comprised 24 Animal Stroop stimuli printed on a
sheet of paper. The stimuli were designed and based on
four images of animals: a cow, a duck, a pig, and a sheep.
In both conditions, the children were asked to name the
animalʼs body. In the conflict condition, the head of the
animal was substituted with the head of another animal.
In the no-conflict condition, the head and the animalʼs
body were matched (Figure 1). All preschoolers named
each of the 24 animal bodies under the no-conflict condition before naming the 24 animal bodies under the conflict condition. RTs and ERs were recorded separately for
the conflict and the no-conflict conditions. The Stroop
interference score was defined as the differences in RTs
or ERs between the two experimental conditions. The
higher Stroop interference scores reflected lower CC
efficiency in these children.
The verbal working memory efficiency was assessed
using the forward and backward digit span tasks from the
WISC-IV (Wechsler, 2003). In these two working memory
tasks, the children listened to a series of discrete digits
and subsequently recalled the series of digits in the same
Volume 26, Number 1

(i.e., forward digit span task) or reverse (i.e., backward
digit span task) order of presentation. In each task, the
children first performed two series of two digits. The series
of digits were subsequently increased by one digit every
two trials. The task was terminated when a child failed
to recall two consecutive series with the same number of
digits. The working memory span (or score) was defined
as the number of correctly recalled digits in the last series.
The forward and backward digit span tasks were used to
assess the ability to maintain (i.e., forward digit span task)
or maintain and manipulate information (i.e., backward
digit span task), respectively, in verbal working memory.
In addition, children performed a battery of nonexecutive tasks: three Piagetian logicomathematical tasks (i.e.,
the number conservation task, the substance conservation
task, and the class inclusion task), a numerical task (i.e.,
the number estimation line task), and a visual task (i.e.,
Navonʼs local–global task). Therefore, childrenʼs performance on these tasks were not analyzed in this study.
MRI Acquisition
We acquired anatomical MRI from the Cyceron biomedical
imaging platform (Caen, France, www.cyceron.fr) on
a 3T MRI scanner (Achieva, Philips Medical System, The
Netherlands), using 3-D T1-weighted spoiled gradient images (field of view = 256 mm, slice thickness =
1.33 mm, 128 slices, matrix size = 192 × 192 voxels).
To reduce motion, provide a positive experience, and
decrease wait times, we obtained MRIs as the children
passively watched a cartoon on an MRI-compatible screen
(Lemaire, Moran, & Swan, 2009).
MRI Analysis
An automated preprocessing step skull-stripped T1 MRIs
and segmented the brain tissues. No spatial normalization
was applied to MRIs to overcome potential bias that may
result from the sulcus shape deformations induced by
the warping process. The cortical folds were automatically
segmented throughout the cortex from the skeleton of
the gray matter/cerebrospinal fluid mask, with the cortical
folds corresponding to the crevasse bottoms of the
“landscape,” the altitude of which is defined by its intensity
on the MRIs. This definition provides a stable and robust
sulcal surface definition that is not affected by variations
in cortical thickness or gray matter/white matter contrast
(Mangin et al., 2004). For each participant, images at each
processing step were visually checked. No segmentation
error was detected. Image analysis was performed with
the Morphologist toolbox using BrainVISA 4.2 software
(brainvisa.info).
ACC Classification
The sulcal pattern of ACC was visually assessed using 3-D,
mesh-based reconstruction of cortical folds to measure

the occurrence and extent of local sulci (e.g., Leonard,
Towler, Welcome, & Chiarello, 2009; Huster, Westerhausen,
Kreuder, Schweiger, & Wittling, 2007; Fornito et al., 2004;
Yucel et al., 2001). ACC sulcal pattern was classified as
“single” or “double parallel” type (Ono et al., 1990) based
on the presence or absence of a PCS (Figure 2). This
3-D approach was used to overcome methodological
issues inherent to the analysis of the sulcal pattern of
ACC from the two-dimensional sagittal slices. The PCS
was defined as the sulcus located dorsal to the cingulate
sulcus with a course clearly parallel to the cingulate
sulcus ( Yucel et al., 2001; Paus et al., 1996). To reduce
the ambiguity from the confluence of the PCS and the
cingulate sulcus with the superior rostral sulcus (Paus
et al., 1996), we determined the anterior limit of the
PCS as the point at which the sulcus extends posteriorly
from an imaginary vertical line running perpendicular
to the line passing through the anterior and posterior
commissures (AC–PC) and parallel to the anterior commissure (Huster et al., 2007; Yucel et al., 2001). The PCS
was considered absent if there were no clearly developed horizontal sulcus elements parallel to the cingulate
sulcus and extending at least 20 mm (interruptions
or gaps in the PCS course was not taken into account
for the length measure). The finer distinction between
“present” and “prominent” PCS ( Yucel et al., 2001; Paus
et al., 1996), leading to three ACC sulcal pattern types,
was not used here because this distinction is based on
the PCS length of adult brains—that is, greater than
20 mm according to Pausʼs classification (Paus et al.,
1996) or greater than 40 mm according to Yucelʼs classification (Yucel et al., 2001) for a prominent PCS (Leonard
et al., 2009). Furthermore, the classification of ACC morphology into five categories by grouping the individual
measurements of PCS in 15-mm steps was also proposed
(Huster et al., 2007). However, the present/prominent or
five categories of ACC sulcal pattern cannot be applied
to characterize ACC sulcal pattern of developing brains,
as brain size and PCS length increase with age. Notably,
the binary classification of ACC sulcal pattern (“single”/
“double parallel” type) used in our study was previously
used in a study on schizophrenia (Fornito, Yucel, et al.,
2006).
Statistical Analysis
We conducted separately 2 (ACC Sulcal Pattern, i.e.,
“symmetric” vs. “asymmetric”) × 2 (Stroop Condition,
i.e., “conflict” vs. “no-conflict”) mixed-design ANOVAs
on the RTs and ERs of the Stroop task. In addition, we
ran a 2 (ACC Sulcal Pattern, i.e., “symmetric” vs. “asymmetric”) × 2 (Working Memory Task, i.e., “forward” vs.
“backward”) mixed-design ANOVA on the scores in
the forward and backward digit span tasks. When we
compared two means, we computed two-tailed t tests
or Welchʼs t tests in cases of unequal variances. For each
analysis, we reported the effect size either in the ANOVA
Cachia et al.

99

Figure 3. Interindividual
variability of ACC sulcal pattern.
Superimposition of the 3-D
mesh-based reconstructions of
the cingulate sulcus (turquoise)
and PCS (blue) for all children
included in the study. Sulci
were represented on the
cortical surface (gray/white
interface). The reconstructions
of the sulci of each child were
linearly aligned in a common
referential (MNI space) for
visualization purpose.

(partial η2) or in terms of the difference of the means
(Cohenʼs d).

RESULTS
Participants were divided into two groups based on the
asymmetry of the sulcal pattern of ACC: 11 children with
symmetrical ACC sulcal pattern—“single” (n = 9) or
“double parallel” (n = 2) type in both hemispheres—
and 8 children with asymmetrical ACC sulcal pattern—
“single” type in the left hemisphere and “double parallel”
type in the right hemisphere (n = 4) or vice versa (n =
4; see Figure 3 for the interindividual variability of ACC
sulcal pattern). These groups were matched for age, sex,
household income as a proxy indicator for socioeconomic
status, scores on the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory
(Oldfield, 1971), and raw scores on the colored progressive matrices of Raven as a proxy indicator for general
intelligence (Raven, Raven, & Court, 1976; see Table 1).
The two-way mixed-design ANOVA in the RTs demonstrated that, irrespective of ACC sulcal pattern, children
needed more time to name the animalsʼ bodies in the
conflict (65.1 ± 28.3 sec) than in the no-conflict (37.5 ±
10.5 sec) conditions, F(1, 17) = 32.57, p < .0001, ηp2 =
.66, revealing a classical Stroop-like interference effect on
the RTs. Furthermore, we found no main effect of ACC
Sulcal Pattern—that is, RTs averaged over the Stroop conditions did not differ between children with symmetric
ACC and children with asymmetric ACC, F(1, 17) = 1.67,

p = .26. Finally, the difference in RTs between the conflict
and the no-conflict conditions was greater for children with
symmetric ACC sulcal pattern (74.2 ± 31.8 sec vs. 37.3 ±
10.8 sec) than for children with asymmetric ACC sulcal
pattern (52.5 ± 17.6 sec vs. 37.7 ± 10.8 sec) as witnessed
by a significant two-way interaction, F(1, 17) = 5.87, p <
.025, ηp2 = .26. Critically, the Stroop interference scores
(i.e., RT difference between the conflict and no-conflict
conditions) were lower; thus, CC efficiency was higher in
children with asymmetrical ACC (14.9 ± 13.2 sec) than in
children with symmetrical ACC (36.8 ± 22.9 sec), Welch-t
(16.32) = 2.64, p < .025, d = 1.17 (Figure 4). ACC asymmetry explained 21% of Stroop interference score variability based on RTs. Finally, we note that children in the
two groups required the same amount of time to name
the animal body in the no-conflict condition (37.3 sec vs.
37.7 sec), t(17) = .06, p = .95 (see Table 2).
A two-way mixed-design ANOVA on the ERs demonstrated a similar pattern of results. Children had more
errors in the conflict (8.1 ± 4.7%) than in the no-conflict
conditions (3.5 ± 3.7%), F(1, 17) = 22.37, p < .0001,
ηp2 = .57; the sulcal pattern of ACC had no main effect
on the ERs, F < 1. Finally, similar to the findings for the
RTs, the difference in ERs between the conflict and the
no-conflict conditions was greater for children with
symmetric ACC sulcal pattern (8.7 ± 4.4% vs. 2.3 ± 3.9%)
than for children with asymmetric ACC sulcal pattern
(7.3 ± 5.3% vs. 5.2 ± 2.9%), as demonstrated by a significant
two-way interaction, F(1, 17) = 5.84, p < .025, ηp2 = .26.

Table 1. Demographic Characteristics of the Sample of Preschoolers (n = 19)

Age (years)
Sex (female/male)

Sym. n = 11

Asym. n = 8

5.48 (0.2)

5.47 (0.15)

5/6

Welch-t/χ2

p

t = 0.10

.91

5/3

χ = 0.12

.73

2

χ = 1.66

.19

2

Household (low/high income)

5/6

6/2

Handedness (Oldfield score)

91.9 (26.8)

94.4 (15.7)

t = .025

.80

Raven (raw score)

27.9 (3.2)

29.3 (1.1)

t = .087

.40

100

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

Volume 26, Number 1

Figure 4. Asymmetry of
ACC and cognitive control
efficiency in preschoolers
(n = 19). Average Stroop
interference scores (RTs and
ERs) in preschoolers with
symmetrical ACC (“single”
type or “double parallel” type
in both hemispheres; light
gray; n = 11) or asymmetrical
ACC (“single” type in the left
hemisphere and “double
parallel” type in the right
hemisphere or vice versa;
hashed; n = 8). Error bars
denote SEM.

Consistent with the results reported on the RTs, the Stroop
interference scores computed on the ERs were lower in
children with asymmetrical ACC sulcal pattern (2.1 ±
3.9%) than in children with symmetrical ACC sulcal pattern
(6.4 ± 3.9%), Welch-t (15.32) = 2.42, p < .05, d = 1.1. As
for the RTs, 21% of the variance of the Stroop interference
score computed on the ERs is explained by the sulcal patTable 2. Mean (M ) and Standard Deviation (SD) of the RTs
and ERs in the Stroop Color Word Task for Children with
Symmetrical (Sym.) and Asymmetrical (Asym.) Sulcal Pattern
of ACC
Sym.

Asym.

M

SD

M

SD

No conflict

37.3

10.8

37.6

10.7

Conflict

74.2

31.8

52.5

17.6

Stroop score

36.8

22.9

14.9

13.4

No conflict

2.3

3.9

5.2

2.9

Conflict

8.7

4.4

7.3

5.3

Stroop score

6.4

3.9

2.1

3.9

RTs (sec)

tern of ACC. As for the RTs, children with asymmetrical
(5.2 ± 2.9%) and symmetrical (2.3 ± 3.9%) sulcal pattern
of ACC committed approximately the same number of
errors in the no-conflict condition, t(17) = 1.79, p = .09.
A 2 (ACC Sulcal Pattern, i.e., symmetric vs. asymmetric) ×
2 (Working Memory Task, i.e., forward vs. backward) mixeddesign ANOVA revealed that irrespective of ACC sulcal
pattern, the scores in the backward digit span tasks were
smaller (M = 2.32 ± .58) than those obtained in the forward
(M = 3.79 ± .92) digit span tasks, F(1, 17) = 30.85, p <
.0001, ηp2 = .65. There was no main effect of ACC Sulcal
Pattern on the scores averaged over the two conditions,
F < 1. Critically, the differences between the forward
and backward scores were similar for children with
symmetric (4.09 ± 1.04 vs. 2.18 ± .41) and asymmetric
(3.38 ± .52 vs. 2.5 ± .76) ACC patterns, as the lack of significant two-way interactions suggests, F(1, 17) = 4.26,
p = .06. Moreover, planned comparisons revealed that
the scores did not differ between the children with symmetric and asymmetric ACC sulcal patterns in the forward, t(17) = 1.77, p = .09, and backward, t(17) = 1.19,
p = .25, digit span tasks.

ERs ( %)

DISCUSSION
As expected, children with an asymmetrical sulcal pattern of ACC are less sensitive to the interference in a
Cachia et al.

101

Stroop-like task than children with a symmetrical sulcal
pattern of ACC. Thus, preschoolersʼ CC efficiency including inhibitory control—that is, the ability to overcome a
cognitive conflict and inhibit a dominant response—is
directly related to their ACC sulcal pattern. Critically,
the effect of the sulcal pattern of ACC on the Stroop
interference score unlikely results from a difference in
the ability to name the animals, given that we found no
difference in the no-conflict condition between the two
groups. Furthermore, we note that analyses of the RTs
and ERs revealed a similar pattern of results, with strong
effect sizes providing evidence (a) of the robustness of
the behavioral effects of ACC sulcal pattern, despite the
sample size, and (b) the lack of speed accuracy trade-off.
Moreover, ACC sulcal pattern had no effect on performances in the forward and backward digit span tasks.
Taken together, the results suggest that ACC sulcal pattern contributes to the ability to resolve conflicts (i.e.,
as in the Animal Stroop task) but did not contribute to
the ability to maintain and manipulate information in
verbal working memory (i.e., as in the forward and the
backward digit span tasks) or manage the increasing difficulty of a task (i.e., the difference between the scores
in the backward and the forward digit span tasks). A
potential limitation of this study is the small sample size,
reflecting the difficulty to conduct brain imaging studies
in young children.
This study focuses on the general construct of executive functions and the inhibition of prepotent responses;
therefore, our findings do not provide information on
the specific cognitive processes affected by ACC sulcal
pattern. Indeed, although ACC is consistently activated
in the Stroop task (Roberts & Hall, 2008; Nee, Wager, &
Jonides, 2007), the precise role of this region remains
elusive (Botvinick, Cohen, & Carter, 2004; MacLeod &
MacDonald, 2000). ACC is critical for monitoring conflicts
(Kerns et al., 2004; Botvinick et al., 2001; Carter et al.,
2000), selecting responses in underdetermined contexts
(Palmer et al., 2001), detecting errors (Braver, Barch, Gray,
Molfese, & Snyder, 2001; Falkenstein, Hoormann, Christ,
& Hohnsbein, 2000; Carter et al., 1998), making rewardbased decisions (Nieuwenhuis, Yeung, Holroyd, Schurger,
& Cohen, 2004; Bush et al., 2002), and encoding cognitive efforts (Rushworth, Walton, Kennerley, & Bannerman,
2004; Botvinick et al., 2001). Additional studies are needed
to determine the precise cognitive process affected by
the morphology of ACC, which in turn might shed light
on the cognitive processes critical to perform the Animal
Stroop task and the role of ACC in supporting these processes. For instance, future researches should study the
effect of ACC sulcal pattern on the performance in different executive tasks tackling different types of executive
processes.
Thus, our results support the hypothesis that CC
efficiency in preschoolers is rooted in early neurodevelopmental processes. The cortical folding patterns are
primarily determined in utero ( Welker, 1988) and are
102

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

robust to changes induced by maturation after birth
and experience-dependent factors (Sun et al., 2012). The
sulcal pattern results from early neurodevelopmental
processes, starting as early as 10 weeks of fetal life, that
shape the cortex anatomy from a smooth lissencephalic
structure to a highly convoluted surface (Welker, 1988).
In particular, the development of ACC sulcal pattern
occurs between 10 and 15 weeks of fetal life (FeessHiggins & Larroche, 1987; Chi, Dooling, & Gilles, 1977).
Such long-term effect of early neurodevelopmental
constraints on the subsequent development of cognitive
capacities is in line with previous studies in adults showing
that variations in ACC sulcal patterns are related to individual differences in CC efficiency (Huster et al., 2009;
Fornito et al., 2004) as well as in four core temperament
dimensions, that is, effortful control, negative affectivity,
surgency, and affiliation (Whittle et al., 2009). An important contribution of our findings is that such long-term
effect was observed for the first time in preschoolers,
namely in the early stages of cognitive and neural development. However, because of the sample size, we could
not statistically determine whether children with a leftward asymmetry of the sulcal pattern of ACC (i.e., PCS
in the left but not in the right hemispheres) have greater
CC efficiency than children with a rightward asymmetry
(i.e., PCS in the left but not in the right hemispheres) as
in adults (Huster et al., 2009; Whittle et al., 2009; Fornito
et al., 2004).1
Several factors contribute to the neurodevelopmental
processes that influence the shape of the folded cerebral
cortex (Mangin, Jouvent, & Cachia, 2010), including structural connectivity through axonal tension forces (Hilgetag
& Barbas, 2006; Van Essen, 1997). These mechanical
constraints lead to a compact layout that optimizes the
transmission of neuronal signals between brain regions
(Klyachko & Stevens, 2003) and thus brain network functioning. In this context, we speculate that the differences
in CC efficiency observed in preschoolers with symmetrical
or asymmetrical ACC might reflect differences in brain
network efficiency because of differences in long-range
(i.e., interhemispheric) and short-range (i.e., intrahemispheric) brain connectivity. Increased cognitive efficiency
in asymmetric brains might be associated with hemispheric
specialization, as it is more efficient to transfer information between close areas within the same hemisphere
rather than between distant areas distributed in the two
hemispheres (Deary, Penke, & Johnson, 2010; Toga &
Thompson, 2003). The association between hemispheric
specialization and the asymmetry of the brain morphology
is supported by studies of the corpus callosum, a large
bundle of interhemispheric fibers, showing that asymmetrical brains have fewer and/or thinner fibers connecting
the two hemispheres relative to more symmetrical brain,
as evidenced by a reduced midsagittal area ( Witelson,
1985) and microstructural integrity measured using diffusion MRI (Putnam, Wig, Grafton, Kelley, & Gazzaniga,
2008). Individuals with no corpus callosum (i.e., complete
Volume 26, Number 1

agenesis) exhibit an intact Stroop interference effect
(Brown, Thrasher, & Paul, 2001), suggesting that the processes involved in performing the Stroop task are highly
lateralized in the brain. If these processes are lateralized,
then it is reasonable to expect that morphological asymmetry in the regions involved in performing the Stroop
task, such as ACC, would produce better efficiency by
reinforcing hemispheric specialization. This hypothesis is
supported by multimodal brain imaging of the cortex
morphology and the white matter connectivity in the
same individuals, revealing that participants with different
sulcal patterns have distinct short-range white matter connectivity (Leonard, Eckert, & Kuldau, 2006). Therefore,
the increased CC efficiency observed in children with
asymmetric ACC in our study might reflect the asymmetry
of the underlying white matter connectivity of ACC.
Faulty executive functions including CC can account for
learning difficulties in children, such as errors, reasoning
biases, and maladjustment, both in cognitive (Borst, Poirel,
Pineau, Cassotti, & Houdé, 2013; Poirel et al., 2012; Houdé,
2000) and social (Steinberg, 2005) domains. Childrenʼs
executive function efficiency actually predicts, for instance, health and professional success later in life (Moffitt
et al., 2011). The predictive nature of childrenʼs executive
functions including CC efficiency on cognitive and social
development may be partly related to the early neurodevelopmental constraints induced by the sulcal pattern
of ACC (Dubois et al., 2008).
However, brain–behavior associations are not fixed in
all cases. For instance, previous studies reported that CC
efficiency, assessed either in a visual discrimination task
(Casey et al., 1997) or in a Flanker task (Fjell et al., 2012),
is related to the cortical surface area of ACC (Fjell et al.,
2012; Casey et al., 1997). Critically, the relationship is
stronger for younger children (under 12 years old) and
decreases linearly with age (Fjell et al., 2012). Because
the cortical surface area of ACC is determined in part
by ACC sulcal pattern (Fornito et al., 2008; Fornito, Whittle,
et al., 2006), the relationship between ACC sulcal pattern
and the CC efficiency reported in our study might vary
with age. Hence, early neuroanatomical constraints might
be overcome during cognitive development by training
CC. Indeed, CC continues to mature from childhood to
adolescence (Luna, 2009; Luna, Garver, Urban, Lazar, &
Sweeney, 2004) and can be modified by training and
practice (Diamond, 2013).
Longitudinal studies in large-scale samples should
investigate the possible interactions between the sulcal
pattern of ACC and intense preschool interventions that
have shown to improve executive functions including
CC efficiency (Diamond et al., 2007). An early objective
assessment of CC in children is critical, given that (a)
executive function training is more beneficial for children
with faulty executive function efficiency (Diamond et al.,
2007) and (b) executive functions can demonstrate improved results in children as early as 4 or 5 years (Moffitt
et al., 2011). Hence, our brain-imaging findings may

ultimately contribute to shape specific educational interventions designed to help children overcome their CC
deficits, particularly those with symmetrical sulcal pattern
of ACC who are at increased risk of developing faulty CC
efficiency.
In conclusion, this study provides the first evidence
that preschoolersʼ CC efficiency is likely associated with
ACC sulcal pattern, thereby suggesting that the brain
shape could result in early constraints on human executive
ability.
Acknowledgments
We thank the CNRS for financial support and the French Board
of Education for their collaboration.
Reprint requests should be sent to Arnaud Cachia, Laboratory
for the Psychology of Child Development and Education, CNRS
U3521, Paris-Descartes University, Alliance for Higher Education
and Research Sorbonne Paris Cité, Sorbonne, 46 rue Saint-Jacques,
75005 Paris, France, or via e-mail: arnaud.cachia@parisdescartes.fr.

Note
1. That said, the pattern of Stroop interference scores suggests
that children with leftward asymmetry (13.1 ± 17.4; n = 4) could
have greater CC efficiency than children with rightward asymmetry (16.7 ± 9.7; n = 4), children with PCS in both hemispheres (24.9 ± 11.5; n = 2), and children with no PCS (39.5 ±
24.4; n = 9).

REFERENCES
Amiez, C., Neveu, R., Warrot, D., Petrides, M., Knoblauch, K.,
& Procyk, E. (2013). The location of feedback-related
activity in the midcingulate cortex is predicted by local
morphology. Journal of Neuroscience, 33, 2217–2228.
Armstrong, E., Schleicher, A., Omran, H., Curtis, M., &
Zilles, K. (1995). The ontogeny of human gyrification.
Cerebral Cortex, 5, 56–63.
Baddeley, A. (2003). Working memory: Looking back and
looking forward. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4,
829–839.
Barkovich, A. J., Guerrini, R., Kuzniecky, R. I., Jackson, G. D.,
& Dobyns, W. B. (2012). A developmental and genetic
classification for malformations of cortical development:
Update 2012. Brain, 135, 1348–1369.
Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control,
executive function, and false belief understanding to
emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten.
Child Development, 78, 647–663.
Borst, G., Poirel, N., Pineau, A., Cassotti, M., & Houdé, O.
(2013). Inhibitory control efficiency in a Piaget-like
class-inclusion task in school-age children and adults:
A developmental negative priming study. Developmental
Psychology, 49, 1366–1374.
Botvinick, M. M. (2007). Conflict monitoring and decision
making: Reconciling two perspectives on anterior cingulate
function. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience,
7, 356–366.
Botvinick, M. M., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Carter, C. S.,
& Cohen, J. D. (2001). Conflict monitoring and cognitive
control. Psychological Review, 108, 624–652.

Cachia et al.

103

Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict
monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 539–546.
Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Gray, J. R., Molfese, D. L., &
Snyder, A. (2001). Anterior cingulate cortex and response
conflict: Effects of frequency, inhibition and errors.
Cerebral Cortex, 11, 825–836.
Brown, W. S., Thrasher, E. D., & Paul, L. K. (2001).
Interhemispheric Stroop effects in partial and complete
agenesis of the corpus callosum. Journal of the
International Neuropsychological Society, 7, 302–311.
Bush, G., Luu, P., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Cognitive and
emotional influences in anterior cingulate cortex.
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 215–222.
Bush, G., Vogt, B. A., Holmes, J., Dale, A. M., Greve, D.,
Jenike, M. A., et al. (2002). Dorsal anterior cingulate cortex:
A role in reward-based decision making. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 99, 523–528.
Carter, C. S., Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Botvinick, M. M., Noll,
D., & Cohen, J. D. (1998). Anterior cingulate cortex, error
detection, and the online monitoring of performance.
Science, 280, 747–749.
Carter, C. S., Macdonald, A. M., Botvinick, M., Ross, L. L.,
Stenger, V. A., Noll, D., et al. (2000). Parsing executive
processes: Strategic vs. evaluative functions of the anterior
cingulate cortex. Proceedings of the National Academy
of Sciences, U.S.A., 97, 1944–1948.
Casey, B. J., Thomas, K. M., Welsh, T. F., Badgaiyan, R. D.,
Eccard, C. H., Jennings, J. R., et al. (2000). Dissociation of
response conflict, attentional selection, and expectancy with
functional magnetic resonance imaging. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 97, 8728–8733.
Casey, B. J., Trainor, R., Giedd, J., Vauss, Y., Vaituzis, C. K.,
Hamburger, S., et al. (1997). The role of the anterior
cingulate in automatic and controlled processes:
A developmental neuroanatomical study. Developmental
Psychobiology, 30, 61–69.
Chi, J. G., Dooling, E. C., & Gilles, F. H. (1977). Gyral
development of the human brain. Annals of Neurology,
1, 86–93.
Deary, I. J., Penke, L., & Johnson, W. (2010). The neuroscience
of human intelligence differences. Nature Reviews
Neuroscience, 11, 201–211.
Dehay, C., Giroud, P., Berland, M., Killackey, H., & Kennedy, H.
(1996). Contribution of thalamic input to the specification
of cytoarchitectonic cortical fields in the primate: Effects of
bilateral enucleation in the fetal monkey on the boundaries,
dimensions, and gyrification of striate and extrastriate cortex.
Journal of Comparative Neurology, 367, 70–89.
Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of
Psychology, 64, 135–168.
Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007).
Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science,
318, 1387–1388.
Diamond, A., & Lee, K. (2011). Interventions shown to
aid executive function development in children 4 to
12 years old. Science, 333, 959–964.
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U.,
& May, A. (2004). Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter
induced by training. Nature, 427, 311–312.
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Kempermann, G., Kuhn, H. G.,
Winkler, J., Buchel, C., et al. (2006). Temporal and spatial
dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive
learning. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 6314–6317.
Dubois, J., Benders, M., Borradori-Tolsa, C., Cachia, A.,
Lazeyras, F., Ha-Vinh Leuchter, R., et al. (2008). Primary
cortical folding in the human newborn: An early marker
of later functional development. Brain, 131, 2028–2041.

104

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

Duckworth, A. L., & Seligman, M. E. (2005). Self-discipline
outdoes IQ in predicting academic performance of
adolescents. Psychological Science, 16, 939–944.
Egner, T., & Hirsch, J. (2005). Cognitive control mechanisms
resolve conflict through cortical amplification of task-relevant
information. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 1784–1790.
Falkenstein, M., Hoormann, J., Christ, S., & Hohnsbein, J.
(2000). ERP components on reaction errors and their
functional significance: A tutorial. Biological Psychology,
51, 87–107.
Feess-Higgins, A., & Larroche, J. (1987). Development of the
human foetal brain. An anatomical atlas. Paris: Masson.
Fjell, A. M., Walhovd, K. B., Brown, T. T., Kuperman, J. M.,
Chung, Y., Hagler, D. J., Jr., et al. (2012). Multimodal imaging
of the self-regulating developing brain. Proceedings of the
National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 109, 19620–19625.
Flook, L., Smalley, S. L., Kitil, M. J., Galla, B. M., KaiserGreenland, S., Locke, J., et al. (2010). Effects of mindful
awareness practices on executive functions in elementary
school children. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 26,
70–95.
Fornito, A., Whittle, S., Wood, S. J., Velakoulis, D., Pantelis, C.,
& Yucel, M. (2006). The influence of sulcal variability on
morphometry of the human anterior cingulate and
paracingulate cortex. Neuroimage, 33, 843–854.
Fornito, A., Wood, S. J., Whittle, S., Fuller, J., Adamson, C.,
Saling, M. M., et al. (2008). Variability of the paracingulate
sulcus and morphometry of the medial frontal cortex:
Associations with cortical thickness, surface area, volume,
and sulcal depth. Human Brain Mapping, 29, 222–236.
Fornito, A., Yucel, M., Wood, S. J., Proffitt, T., McGorry,
P. D., Velakoulis, D., et al. (2006). Morphology of
the paracingulate sulcus and executive cognition in
schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 88, 192–197.
Fornito, A., Yucel, M., Wood, S., Stuart, G. W., Buchanan,
J. A., Proffitt, T., et al. (2004). Individual differences in
anterior cingulate/paracingulate morphology are related
to executive functions in healthy males. Cerebral Cortex,
14, 424–431.
Gathercole, S. E., Pickering, S. J., Ambridge, B., & Wearing, H.
(2004). The structure of working memory from 4 to 15 years
of age. Developmental Psychology, 40, 177–190.
Giedd, J. N., Lalonde, F. M., Celano, M. J., White, S. L.,
Wallace, G. L., Lee, N. R., et al. (2009). Anatomical brain
magnetic resonance imaging of typically developing
children and adolescents. Journal of the American
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 48, 465–470.
Gogtay, N., Giedd, J. N., Lusk, L., Hayashi, K. M., Greenstein, D.,
Vaituzis, A. C., et al. (2004). Dynamic mapping of human
cortical development during childhood through early
adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of
Sciences, U.S.A., 101, 8174–8179.
Hilgetag, C. C., & Barbas, H. (2006). Role of mechanical
factors in the morphology of the primate cerebral cortex.
PLOS Computational Biology, 2, e22.
Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I., & Kramer, A. F. (2008).
Be smart, exercise your heart: Exercise effects on brain
and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9, 58–65.
Holmes, J., Gathercole, S. E., & Dunning, D. L. (2009). Adaptive
training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working
memory in children. Developmental Science, 12, F9–F15.
Houdé, O. (2000). Inhibition and cognitive development:
Object, number, categorization, and reasoning. Cognitive
Development, 15, 63–73.
Huster, R. J., Enriquez-Geppert, S., Pantev, C., & Bruchmann, M.
(2012). Variations in midcingulate morphology are related
to ERP indices of cognitive control. Brain Structure
& Function. Epub ahead of print.

Volume 26, Number 1

Huster, R. J., Westerhausen, R., & Herrmann, C. S. (2011).
Sex differences in cognitive control are associated with
midcingulate and callosal morphology. Brain Structure &
Function, 215, 225–235.
Huster, R. J., Westerhausen, R., Kreuder, F., Schweiger, E., &
Wittling, W. (2007). Morphologic asymmetry of the human
anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroimage, 34, 888–895.
Huster, R. J., Wolters, C., Wollbrink, A., Schweiger, E.,
Wittling, W., Pantev, C., et al. (2009). Effects of anterior
cingulate fissurization on cognitive control during Stroop
interference. Human Brain Mapping, 30, 1279–1289.
Hyde, K. L., Lerch, J., Norton, A., Forgeard, M., Winner, E.,
Evans, A. C., et al. (2009). Musical training shapes structural
brain development. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 3019–3025.
Kerns, J. G., Cohen, J. D., MacDonald, A. W., III, Cho, R. Y.,
Stenger, V. A., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Anterior cingulate
conflict monitoring and adjustments in control. Science,
303, 1023–1026.
Klyachko, V. A., & Stevens, C. F. (2003). Connectivity
optimization and the positioning of cortical areas.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences,
U.S.A., 100, 7937–7941.
Lakes, K. D., & Hoyt, W. I. (2004). Promoting self-regulation
through school-based martial arts training. Journal of
Applied Developmental Psychology, 25, 283–302.
Lemaire, C., Moran, G. R., & Swan, H. (2009). Impact of
audio/visual systems on pediatric sedation in magnetic
resonance imaging. Journal of Magnetic Resonance
Imaging, 30, 649–655.
Leonard, C. M., Eckert, M. A., & Kuldau, J. M. (2006). Exploiting
human anatomical variability as a link between genome
and cognome. Genes, Brain and Behavior, 5(Suppl. 1),
64–77.
Leonard, C. M., Towler, S., Welcome, S., & Chiarello, C. (2009).
Paracingulate asymmetry in anterior and midcingulate
cortex: Sex differences and the effect of measurement
technique. Brain Structure & Function, 213, 553–569.
Luna, B. (2009). Developmental changes in cognitive control
through adolescence. Advances in Child Development
and Behavior, 37, 233–278.
Luna, B., Garver, K. E., Urban, T. A., Lazar, N. A., & Sweeney,
J. A. (2004). Maturation of cognitive processes from late
childhood to adulthood. Child Development, 75, 1357–1372.
Mackey, A. P., Hill, S. S., Stone, S. I., & Bunge, S. A. (2011).
Differential effects of reasoning and speed training in
children. Developmental Science, 14, 582–590.
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the
Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin,
109, 163–203.
MacLeod, C. M., Dodd, M. D., Sheard, E. D., Wilson, D. E., &
Bibi, U. (2003). In opposition to inhibition. In B. H. Ross
(Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation ( Vol. 43,
pp. 163–214). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.
MacLeod, C. M., & MacDonald, P. A. (2000). Interdimensional
interference in the Stroop effect: Uncovering the cognitive
and neural anatomy of attention. Trends in Cognitive
Sciences, 4, 383–391.
Mangin, J. F., Jouvent, E., & Cachia, A. (2010). In-vivo
measurement of cortical morphology: Means and meanings.
Current Opinion in Neurology, 23, 359–367.
Mangin, J. F., Riviere, D., Cachia, A., Duchesnay, E., Cointepas, Y.,
Papadopoulos-Orfanos, D., et al. (2004). A framework to
study the cortical folding patterns. Neuroimage, 23(Suppl. 1),
S129–S138.
Matthews, S. C., Paulus, M. P., Simmons, A. N., Nelesen, R. A.,
& Dimsdale, J. E. (2004). Functional subdivisions within
anterior cingulate cortex and their relationship to autonomic
nervous system function. Neuroimage, 22, 1151–1156.

Moffitt, T. E., Arseneault, L., Belsky, D., Dickson, N., Hancox,
R. J., Harrington, H., et al. (2011). A gradient of childhood
self-control predicts health, wealth, and public safety.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A.,
108, 2693–2698.
Molko, N., Cachia, A., Riviere, D., Mangin, J. F., Bruandet, M.,
Le Bihan, D., et al. (2003). Functional and structural
alterations of the intraparietal sulcus in a developmental
dyscalculia of genetic origin. Neuron, 40, 847–858.
Nee, D. E., Wager, T. D., & Jonides, J. (2007). Interference
resolution: Insights from a meta-analysis of neuroimaging
tasks. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 7,
1–17.
Nieuwenhuis, S., Yeung, N., Holroyd, C. B., Schurger, A.,
& Cohen, J. D. (2004). Sensitivity of electrophysiological
activity from medial frontal cortex to utilitarian and
performance feedback. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 741–747.
Oldfield, R. C. (1971). The assessment and analysis of
handedness: The Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia,
9, 97–113.
Ono, M., Kubik, S., & Abarnathey, C. D. (1990). Atlas of the
cerebral sulci. New York: Thieme.
Palmer, E. D., Rosen, H. J., Ojemann, J. G., Buckner, R. L.,
Kelley, W. M., & Petersen, S. E. (2001). An event-related
fMRI study of overt and covert word stem completion.
Neuroimage, 14, 182–193.
Pardo, J. V., Pardo, P. J., Janer, K. W., & Raichle, M. E. (1990).
The anterior cingulate cortex mediates processing selection
in the Stroop attentional conflict paradigm. Proceedings of
the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A., 87, 256–259.
Paus, T., Tomaiuolo, F., Otaky, N., MacDonald, D., Petrides, M.,
Atlas, J., et al. (1996). Human cingulate and paracingulate
sulci: Pattern, variability, asymmetry, and probabilistic
map. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 207–214.
Petersen, S. E., & Posner, M. I. (2012). The attention system
of the human brain: 20 Years after. Annual Review of
Neuroscience. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro062111-150525.
Poirel, N., Borst, G., Simon, G., Rossi, S., Cassotti, M., Pineau, A.,
et al. (2012). Number conservation is related to childrenʼs
prefrontal inhibitory control: An fMRI study of a Piagetian
task. PLoS One, 7:e40802.
Putnam, M. C., Wig, G. S., Grafton, S. T., Kelley, W. M., &
Gazzaniga, M. S. (2008). Structural organization of the
corpus callosum predicts the extent and impact of cortical
activity in the nondominant hemisphere. Journal of
Neuroscience, 28, 2912–2918.
Rakic, P. (2004). Neuroscience. Genetic control of cortical
convolutions. Science, 303, 1983–1984.
Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (1998). Manual for Ravenʼs
progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Section 2:
The coloured progressive matrices. San Antonio, Texas:
Harcourt Assessment.
Roberts, K. L., & Hall, D. A. (2008). Examining a supramodal
network for conflict processing: A systematic review and
novel functional magnetic resonance imaging data for
related visual and auditory Stroop tasks. Journal of
Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1063–1078.
Rueda, M. R., Rothbart, M. K., McCandliss, B. D., Saccomanno, L.,
& Posner, M. I. (2005). Training, maturation, and genetic
influences on the development of executive attention.
Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, U.S.A.,
102, 14931–14936.
Rushworth, M. F., Walton, M. E., Kennerley, S. W., &
Bannerman, D. M. (2004). Action sets and decisions in
the medial frontal cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences,
8, 410–417.
Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development
in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9, 69–74.

Cachia et al.

105

Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal
reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18, 643–662.
Sun, Z. Y., Kloppel, S., Riviere, D., Perrot, M., Frackowiak, R.,
Siebner, H., et al. (2012). The effect of handedness on the
shape of the central sulcus. Neuroimage, 60, 332–339.
Takeuchi, H., Taki, Y., Sassa, Y., Hashizume, H., Sekiguchi, A.,
Nagase, T., et al. (2012). Regional gray and white matter
volume associated with Stroop interference: Evidence from
voxel-based morphometry. Neuroimage, 59, 2899–2907.
Toga, A. W., & Thompson, P. M. (2003). Mapping brain
asymmetry. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 37–48.
Van Essen, D. C. (1997). A tension-based theory of
morphogenesis and compact wiring in the central
nervous system. Nature, 385, 313–318.
Vogt, B. (2009). Cingulate neurobiology and disease.
Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for
Children-Fourth Edition. Administration and scoring
manual. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, Inc.
Welker, W. (1988). Why does cerebral cortex fissure and
fold? Cerebral Cortex, 8B, 3–135.
Westlye, L. T., Grydeland, H., Walhovd, K. B., & Fjell, A. M.
(2011). Associations between regional cortical thickness

106

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience

and attentional networks as measured by the attention
network test. Cerebral Cortex, 21, 345–356.
White, T., Su, S., Schmidt, M., Kao, C. Y., & Sapiro, G.
(2010). The development of gyrification in childhood
and adolescence. Brain and Cognition, 72, 36–45.
Whittle, S., Allen, N. B., Fornito, A., Lubman, D. I., Simmons,
J. G., Pantelis, C., et al. (2009). Variations in cortical
folding patterns are related to individual differences
in temperament. Psychiatry Research, 172, 68–74.
Witelson, S. F. (1985). The brain connection: The corpus
callosum is larger in left-handers. Science, 229, 665–668.
Wright, I., Waterman, M., Prescott, H., & Murdoch-Eaton, D.
(2003). A new Stroop-like measure of inhibitory function
development: Typical developmental trends. Journal of
Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 561–575.
Yucel, M., Stuart, G. W., Maruff, P., Velakoulis, D., Crowe, S. F.,
Savage, G., et al. (2001). Hemispheric and gender-related
differences in the gross morphology of the anterior
cingulate/paracingulate cortex in normal volunteers:
An MRI morphometric study. Cerebral Cortex, 11, 17–25.
Zilles, K., Palomero-Gallagher, N., & Amunts, K. (2013).
Development of cortical folding during evolution and
ontogeny. Trends in Neurosciences, 36, 275–284.

Volume 26, Number 1


Aperçu du document jocn_a_00459.pdf - page 1/11

 
jocn_a_00459.pdf - page 2/11
jocn_a_00459.pdf - page 3/11
jocn_a_00459.pdf - page 4/11
jocn_a_00459.pdf - page 5/11
jocn_a_00459.pdf - page 6/11
 







Télécharger le fichier (PDF)





Documents récents du même auteur


HENRI   19 juillet 2016
IDEE   19 juillet 2016
CAFE   19 juillet 2016
PEAU   19 juillet 2016
NOVLANGUE   19 juillet 2016
AVC   19 juillet 2016
DAMIEN   19 juillet 2016
JET LAG   19 juillet 2016
A Putative Human Pheromon1   19 juillet 2016
JORDI LLORENS   19 juillet 2016
reactive and proactive aggression   17 juillet 2016
Holy Warriors Exploring the Psychological Processe   17 juillet 2016
baillement orl (1)   6 juillet 2016
Bienfait de la randonnée   6 juin 2016
pleine lune et sommeil   21 avril 2016
Past experience   18 avril 2016
GENERATION AND TRANSPLANTATION NEURONES 3D   25 mars 2016
Musiq&Parf LaCelle 05-03-2016   6 mars 2016
HFEA approval for new   27 février 2016
CRISP CHINE 2   27 février 2016

Sur le même sujet..







Ce fichier a été mis en ligne par un utilisateur du site Fichier PDF. Identifiant unique du document: 00206789.
⚠️  Signaler un contenu illicite
Pour plus d'informations sur notre politique de lutte contre la diffusion illicite de contenus protégés par droit d'auteur, consultez notre page dédiée.